Survey of the Context-dependent learning hypothesis in badminton service in three levels of skill

Document Type : Research Paper

Abstract

Background&Purpose: Context-dependent learning is a specificity of practice that the performance is better on a task when the environmental context that is present during acquisition, then is reinstated during testing. The present study explored influence of visual context information on badminton service skill and in addition examined whether the amount of practice affects such context-dependence.
Methodology: Three groups of badminton players with three skill levels (less skill, skilled, expert) were selected randomly. They performed two types of badminton service (the high & backhand short service) in two environmental conditions (the normal court & without standard lines court). Each of the groups was 12 players (mean of age 24/33±2/51) that performed 90 services of each type of badminton service. The Paired-Samples t-test was used to compare the performance accuracy in two environmental conditions.
Results: Results showed that there was no significant difference for less skill group performance on two courts, but accuracy of performance of the expert & skilled groups in the normal court significantly was better than without lines court.
Conclusion: These findings support the Context-dependent learning hypothesis and demonstrate that this context-dependence induces with increase the practice.

Keywords

Main Subjects


1.Schmidt RA, Lee TD. Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis. In: Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. [aRP, HNZ], (2005).
2.Smith SM, Vela E. Environmental context-dependent memory: A review and meta-analysis. Psychonomic bulletin & review. 2001;8(2):203-20.
3. Wright DL, Shea CH. Contextual dependencies in motor skills. Memory & Cognition. 1991;19(4):361-70.
4.Abrahamse EL, Verwey WB. Context dependent learning in the serial RT task. Psychological research. 2008;72(4):397-404.
5. Godden DR, Baddeley AD. Context‐dependent memory in two natural environments: On land and underwater. British Journal of psychology. 1975;66(3):325-31.
6.Anderson T, Wright DL, Immink MA. Contextual dependencies during perceptual-motor skill performance: Influence of task difficulty. Memory. 1998;6(2):207-21.
7.Ruitenberg MF, Abrahamse EL, De Kleine E, Verwey WB. Context-dependent motor skill: perceptual processing in memory-based sequence production. Experimental brain research. 2012;222(1-2):31-40.
8.Ruitenberg MF, De Kleine E, Van der Lubbe RH, Verwey WB, Abrahamse EL. Context-dependent motor skill and the role of practice. Psychological research. 2012;76(6):812-20.
9.Shea CH, Wright DL. Contextual dependencies: Influence on response latency. Memory. 1995;3(1):81-95.
10.Healy AF, Wohldmann EL, Parker JT, Bourne LE. Skill training, retention, and transfer: The effects of a concurrent secondary task. Memory & Cognition. 2005;33(8):1457-71.
11.Tulving E, Thomson DM. Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological review. 1973;80(5):352.
12.Smith SM. Background music and context-dependent memory. The American journal of psychology. 1985:591-603.
13.Eich JE. The cue-dependent nature of state-dependent retrieval. Memory & Cognition. 1980;8(2):157-73.
14.Eich E. Context, memory, and integrated item/context imagery. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1985;11(4):764.
15.Karniel A, Mussa-Ivaldi F. Does the motor control system use multiple models and context switching to cope with a variable environment? Experimental Brain Research. 2002;143(4):520-4.
16.Krouchev NI, Kalaska JF. Context-dependent anticipation of different task dynamics: rapid recall of appropriate motor skills using visual cues. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2003;89(2):1165-75.
17.Rao AK, Shadmehr R. Contextulal cues facilitate learning of multiple models of arm dynamics. Society for Neuroscience Abstract, 2001.
18.Cohn JV, DiZio P, Lackner JR. Reaching during virtual rotation: context specific compensations for expected coriolis forces. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2000;83(6):3230-40.
19.Gandolfo F, Mussa-Ivaldi F, Bizzi E. Motor learning by field approximation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1996;93(9):3843-6.
20.Stöckel T, Fries U. Motor adaptation in complex sports–The influence of visual context information on the adaptation of the three-point shot to altered task demands in expert basketball players. Journal of sports sciences. 2013;31(7):750-8.
21.Magnuson CE, Wright DL, Verwey WB. Changes in the incidental context impacts search but not loading of the motor buffer. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A. 2004;57(5):935-51.
22. Lucke S, Lachnit H, Koenig S, Uengoer M. The informational value of contexts affects context-dependent learning. Learning & behavior. 2013 Sep 1;41(3):285-97.
23. Lee YY, Winstein CJ, Gordon J, Petzinger GM, Zelinski EM, Fisher BE. Context-dependent learning in people with Parkinson's disease. Journal of motor behavior. 2016 May 3;48(3):240-8.
24Smith SM, Glenberg A, Bjork RA. Environmental context and human memory. Memory & Cognition. 1978;6(4):342-53.
25.Wright DL, Shea CH, Li YH, Whitacre C. Contextual dependencies during perceptual-motor skill acquisition: Gone but not forgotten! Memory. 1996;4(1):91-108.
26. Gershman SJ. Context-dependent learning and causal structure. Psychonomic bulletin & review. 2017 Apr 1;24(2):557-65.
27.Verwey WB. Evidence for a multistage model of practice in a sequential movement task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1999;25(6):1693.
28.Verwey WB. Diminished motor skill development in elderly: indications for limited motor chunk use. Acta psychologica. 2010;134(2):206-14.
29.Hikosaka O, Nakahara H, Rand MK, Sakai K, Lu X, Nakamura K, et al. Parallel neural networks for learning sequential procedures. Trends in neurosciences. 1999;22(10):
464-71.
30.Schvaneveldt RW, Gomez RL. Attention and probabilistic sequence learning. Psychological Research. 1998;61(3): 90-175.
31.Verwey WB, Abrahamse EL, De Kleine E. Cognitive processing in new and practiced discrete keying sequences. Frontiers in psychology. 2010;1:32.
32.Verwey WB, Abrahamse EL, Ruitenberg MF, Jiménez L, de Kleine E. Motor skill learning in the middle-aged: limited development of motor chunks and explicit sequence knowledge. Psychological research. 2011;75(5):406-22.
33Abdoshahi M, Farokhi A, Jabery M.AA, S.M.K VM. Specify the especial skill in backhand short badminton serve: A challenge to schema theory to schema theory. Research in Sport Management & Motor Behavior Kharazmi University. 2013;Third Year, Volume 5.
34.Hadavi F. Measurment and Evaluation in Physical Education. 5 ed. Tehran: Kharazmi University; 2009
35.Ericsson KA, Lehmann AC. Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence of maximal adaptation to task constraints. Annual review of psychology. 1996;47(1):273-305.
36.Hommel B. Action control according to TEC (theory of event coding). Psychological Research PRPF. 2009;73(4):512-26
37.Mann DT, Williams AM, Ward P, Janelle CM. Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 2007;29(4):457.
38.Jiménez L, Vaquero JM, Lupiáñez J. Qualitative differences between implicit and explicit sequence learning. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2006;32(3):475.
39.Magill RA. Motor learning and control: Concepts and applications. New York: McGraw-Hill; 2011.
40. Lee YY, Winstein CJ, Fisher BE. Role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in context‐dependent motor performance. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;43(7):954-60.