The role of associative and dissociative attentional focus with internal and external dimensions on blood lactate and Borg Ratings of Perceived Exertion Scale
Background and Purpose: Since the role of associative and dissociative attentional focus in manipulating various constraints of the task and individual has not been clarified yet, the present study aimed to examine the role of associative and dissociative attentional focus with internal and external dimensions on blood lactate and Borg Ratings of Perceived Exertion Scale in beginners. Methodology: The method of the present study is semi-experimental in terms of practical purpose and based on the method of data collection. The statistical population of this study was all beginner females aged 18 to 30 years old at the Shahid Beheshti University of Tehran. Among them, twelve females were selected as the participants to run on the treadmill. For this purpose, the maximum speed (Vmax) of each participant was measured. The speed used in each of the test conditions (internal associative (attention to muscle), external associative (counting the running steps), internal dissociative (counting down the numbers), and external dissociative (attention to video)) was 70% of the maximum speed of each individual. During running, the questionnaire of Borg Ratings of Perceived Exertion Scale was used. After running for one minute in each condition, blood samples were taken to measure the participants’ blood lactate Results: The results of one-way ANOVA revealed that dissociative attentional focus with internal/external dimension resulted in lower blood lactate and lower Borg Ratings of Perceived Exertion Scale which caused higher running economy in beginners than associative attentional focus. Conclusion: Based on the results of the present study, dissociative attention is the most economical method in running for beginners.
Brick N, MacIntyre T, Campbell M. Attentional focus in endurance activity: new paradigms and future directions. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 2014;7(1):106-34.
Wulf G. Attentional focus and motor learning: a review of 15 years. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 2013;6(1):77-104.
Morgan WP, Horstman DH, Cymerman A, Stokes J. Facilitation of physical performance by means of a cognitive strategy. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 1983;7(3):251-64.
Morgan WP, Pollock ML. Psychologic characterization of the elite distance runner. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1977;301(1):382-403.
Gill D, Williams L, Reifsteck E. Psychological dynamics of sport and exercise: Human Kinetics; 2017.
Connolly C, Janelle C. Attentional strategies in rowing: Performance, perceived exertion, and gender considerations. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology. 2003;15(3):195-212.
Beilock SL, Bertenthal BI, McCoy AM, Carr TH. Haste does not always make waste: Expertise, direction of attention, and speed versus accuracy in performing sensorimotor skills. Psychonomic bulletin & review. 2004;11(2):373-9.
Beilock SL, Carr TH, MacMahon C, Starkes JL. When paying attention becomes counterproductive: impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 2002;8(1):6.
Castaneda B, Gray R. Effects of focus of attention on baseball batting performance in players of differing skill levels. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 2007;29(1):60-77
Kaplan S. The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework. Journal of environmental psychology. 1995;15(3):169-82.
Kaplan, R. and Kaplan, S., 1989. The experience of nature: A psychological perspective. CUP Archive.
Ohly H, White MP, Wheeler BW, Bethel A, Ukoumunne OC, Nikolaou V, Garside R. Attention restoration theory: A systematic review of the attention restoration potential of exposure to natural environments. Journal of Toxicology and Environmental Health, Part B. 2016 Oct 2;19(7):305-43.
Kaplan S, Berman MG. Directed attention as a common resource for executive functioning and self-regulation. Perspectives on psychological science. 2010;5(1):43-57.
Berman MG, Kross E, Krpan KM, Askren MK, Burson A, Deldin PJ, et al. Interacting with nature improves cognition and affect for individuals with depression. Journal of affective disorders. 2012;140(3):300-5.
Pennebaker JW, Lightner JM. Competition of internal and external information in an exercise setting. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1980;39(1):165.
Roe JJ, Aspinall PA, Mavros P, Coyne R. Engaging the brain: the impact of natural versus urban scenes using novel EEG methods in an experimental setting. Environ Sci. 2013;1(2):93-104.
Ekkekakis P. Pleasure and displeasure from the body: Perspectives from exercise. Cognition & Emotion. 2003;17(2):213-39.
Brewer BW, Buman MP. Attentional focus and endurance performance: review and theoretical integration. Kinesiol Slov. 2006;12(2):82-97.
Leventhal H, Everhart D. Emotion, pain, and physical illness. Emotions in personality and psychopathology: Springer; 1979. p. 261-99.
Baden D, McLean T, Tucker R, Noakes T, Gibson ASC. Effect of anticipation during unknown or unexpected exercise duration on rating of perceived exertion, affect, and physiological function. British Journal of Sports Medicine. 2005;39(10):742-6.
Tenenbaum G. A social-cognitive perspective of perceived exertion and exertion tolerance. Handbook of sport psychology. 2001;2:810-20.
Hutchinson JC, Tenenbaum G. Attention focus during physical effort: The mediating role of task intensity. Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 2007;8(2):233-45.
Masters KS, Ogles BM. Associative and dissociative cognitive strategies in exercise and running: 20 years later, what do we know? The Sport Psychologist. 1998;12(3):253-70.
Wulf G, Su J. An external focus of attention enhances golf shot accuracy in beginners and experts. Research quarterly for exercise and sport. 2007;78(4):384-9.
Masters R, Maxwell J. The theory of reinvestment. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 2008;1(2):1. 60-83.
Gill DL, Strom EH. The effect of attentional focus on performance of an endurance task. International Journal of Sport Psychology. 1985.
Morgan W, O'Connor P, Sparling P, Pate R. Psychological characterization of the elite female distance runner. International Journal of Sports Medicine. 1987;8(S 2):S124-S31.
Couture RT, Jerome W, Tihanyi J. Can associative and dissociative strategies affect the swimming performance of recreational swimmers? The Sport Psychologist. 1999;13(3):334-43.
Schücker L, Hagemann N, Strauss B, Völker K. The effect of attentional focus on running economy. Journal of sports sciences. 2009 Oct 1;27(12):1241-8.
Conley DL, Krahenbuhl GS. Running economy and distance running performance of highly trained athletes. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1980 Jan 1;12(5):357-60.
Wedderburn C, Wear H, Brown J, Mason SJ, Barker RA, Hodges J, Williams-Gray C. The utility of the Cambridge Behavioural Inventory in neurodegenerative disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 2008 May 1;79(5):500-3.
Lay, B. S., Sparrow, W. A., & O’Dwyer, N. J. (2005). The metabolic and cognitive energy costs of stabilising a high-energy interlimb coordination task. Human Movement Science, 24, 833– 848.
Sparrow, W. A., & Newel, K. M. (1998). Metabolic energy expenditure and the regulation of movement economy. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 5, 173–196.
Noakes, T. D., Myburgh, K. H., & Schall, R. J. J. o. s. s. (1990). Peak treadmill running velocity during the V O2 max test predicts running performance. 8(1), 35-45.
McNevin NH, Shea CH, Wulf G. Increasing the distance of an external focus of attention enhances learning. Psychological research. 2003;67(1):22-9.
Maxwell JP, Masters R. External versus internal focus instructions: Is the learner paying attention? International Journal of Applied Sports Sciences. 2002;14(2).
Poolton JM, Maxwell J, Masters R, Raab M. Benefits of an external focus of attention: Common coding or conscious processing? Journal of sports sciences. 2006;24(1):89-99.
Schücker L, Anheier W, Hagemann N, Strauss B, Völker K. On the optimal focus of attention for efficient running at high intensity. Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology. 2013;2(3):207.
Johnson J, Siegel D. Active vs. passive attentional manipulation and multidimensional perceptions of exercise intensity. Canadian journal of sport sciences= Journal canadien des sciences du sport. 1987;12(1):41-5.
Stanley CT, Pargman D, Tenenbaum G. The effect of attentional coping strategies on perceived exertion in a cycling task. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology. 2007;19(3):352-63.
Bachman AD, Brewer BW, Petitpas AJ. Situation specificity of cognitions during running: Replication and extension. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology. 1997;9(2):204-11.
Masters KS, Lambert MJ. The relations between cognitive coping strategies, reasons for running, injury, and performance of marathon runners. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology. 1989;11(2):161-70.
Morgan WP, O'Connor PJ, Ellickson KA, Bradley PW. Personality structure, mood states, and performance in elite male distance runners. International Journal of Sport Psychology. 1988.
Morgan W, O'Connor P, Sparling P, Pate R. Psychological characterization of the elite female distance runner. International Journal of Sports Medicine. 1987;8(S 2):S124-S31.
Schomer HH, Connolly MJ. Cognitive strategies used by marathoners in each quartile of a training run. South African Journal for Research in Sport, Physical Education and Recreation. 2002;24(1):87-100.
Delignières D, Brisswalter J, Legros P. Influence of physical exercise on choice reaction time in sports experts: the mediating role of resource allocation. Journal of Human Movement Studies. 1994;27(4):173-88.
Neumann DL, Brown J. The effect of attentional focus strategy on physiological and motor performance during a sit-up exercise. Journal of Psychophysiology. 2013;27(1):7.
Lohse K, Sherwood DE. Defining the focus of attention: effects of attention on perceived exertion and fatigue. Frontiers in psychology. 2011;2:332.
Cioffi D. Sensory awareness versus sensory impression: Affect and attention interact to produce somatic meaning. Cognition & Emotion. 1991;5(4): 94-275.
Vance J, Wulf G, Töllner T, McNevin N, Mercer JJJomb. EMG activity as a function of the performer's focus of attention. 2004;36(4):450-9.
aghdaei, M., farsi, A., & khalaji, M. (2022). The role of associative and dissociative attentional focus with internal and external dimensions on blood lactate and Borg Ratings of Perceived Exertion Scale. Journal of Sport Management and Motor Behavior, 18(36), 175-159. doi: 10.22080/jsmb.2020.15167.2982
MLA
mahin aghdaei; Alireza farsi; maryam khalaji. "The role of associative and dissociative attentional focus with internal and external dimensions on blood lactate and Borg Ratings of Perceived Exertion Scale". Journal of Sport Management and Motor Behavior, 18, 36, 2022, 175-159. doi: 10.22080/jsmb.2020.15167.2982
HARVARD
aghdaei, M., farsi, A., khalaji, M. (2022). 'The role of associative and dissociative attentional focus with internal and external dimensions on blood lactate and Borg Ratings of Perceived Exertion Scale', Journal of Sport Management and Motor Behavior, 18(36), pp. 175-159. doi: 10.22080/jsmb.2020.15167.2982
VANCOUVER
aghdaei, M., farsi, A., khalaji, M. The role of associative and dissociative attentional focus with internal and external dimensions on blood lactate and Borg Ratings of Perceived Exertion Scale. Journal of Sport Management and Motor Behavior, 2022; 18(36): 175-159. doi: 10.22080/jsmb.2020.15167.2982