The effect of Contextual Interference and Practice Type on the Consolidation of Elderly Motor Memory: An Examination of Cognitive Processes of Motor Imaging and Contextual Interference

Document Type : Research Paper

Authors

1 alzahra university

2 .

3 alzahra universty

Abstract

Background Objectives: Contextual interference and motor imagery are variables that affect motor learning. The purpose of this study was to investigate the effect of type of interference and practice on motor memory consolidation in the elderly in order to investigate the cognitive processes of motor imagery and contextual interference.
Methods & Materials: The statistical population of the present study was elderly 80-60 year old men and women in Qazvin city. Participants completed questionnaires of individual profile, Edinburgh handicapping, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality, motor imagery/version 3. According to the criteria for entering the research, 80 elderly people who did not experience the pass and dribble were selected as a statistical sample. The research included pre-test, acquisition, and retention test. The subjects were randomly assigned into 8 groups (Blocked physical, Randomized physical, Self-regulated physical, Yoked with Self-regulated physical, Blocked physical combined with motor imagery, Randomized physical combined with motor imagery, Self-regulated physical combined with motor imagery, Yoked physical combined with motor imagery). All participants were generally trained correctly in the pass and dribble basketball, then in the pre-test, they performed 10 attempts with the arrangement (2A2B). At the acquisition, the subjects of each group, according to their Scheduled practice (a total of 8 blocks of 10 attempts), practiced pass and dribble basketball skills. Retention test was performed 24 hours after the last acquisition block training session. Results: The results of covariance analysis with repeated measure showed that the self-regulated physical combined with motor imagery group in the acquisition and retention test had the best performance and the physical training group was weaker than other groups. The performance of the combination group with random arrangement was better than the combination group with blocked arrangement.

Keywords


  1. Andrieux, M., Boutin, A., & Thon, B. (2015). "Self-control of task difficulty during early practice promotes motor skill learning". Journal of motor behavior, 48(1): 57-65.
  2. Bae, Y.H., Ko, Y., Ha, H., Ahn, S.Y., Lee, W., Lee, S.M. (2015). An efficacy study on improving balance and gait in subacute stroke patients by balance training with additional motor imagery: a pilot study. Journal of physical therapy science. 27(10):3245-48.
  3. Beisteiner, R., Hollinger, Pp., Lindinger, G., Lang, W., & Berthoz, A. (1995). Mental representations of movements. Brain potentials associated with imagination of hand movements. Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology, 96, 183-193.
  4. Brown, R.M., Robertson, E.M., Press, D.Z. (2009). Sequence skill acquisition and offline learning in normal aging. Plos one 4, e6683.
  5. Cairney, S.A., Guttesen, A.V., El Marj, N., Staresina, B.P. (2018). Memory Consolidation Is Linked to Spindle-Mediated Information Processing during Sleep.Curr Biol; 28 (6): 948-954.
  6. Cantarero, G., Tang, B., O’malley, R., Salas, R., Celnik, P. (2013). Motor learning interference is proportional to occlusion of ltp-like plasticity. Journal Neuro science, 33, 4634e4641.
  7. Centeno, C., Medeiros, D., Beck, M.M., Lugassy, L., Gonzalez, D.F., Nepveu, F., Roig, M. (2018).  The effects of aging on cortico-spinal excitability and motor memory consolidation. Neurobiol Aging,70: 254-264.
  8. Coats, R.O., Snapp-Childs, W., Wilson, A.D., Bingham, G.P. (2013). Perceptuomotor learning rate declines by half from 20s to 70/80s. Exp. Brain res, 225, 75e84.
  9. Coelho, C. J., Nusbaum, H. C., Rosenbaum, D. A., & Fenn, K. M. (2012). Imagined actions aren’t just weak actions: task variability promotes skill learning in physical practice but not in mental practice. Journal of experimental psychology: learning, memory, and cognition, 38, 1759–1764.
  10. Curnow D, Cobbin D, Wyndham J, Choy Sb. (2009). Altered motor control, posture and the Pilates method of exercise prescription. Bodywork and movement therapies, 13(1):104-11.
  11. Debarnot, U., Abichou, K., Kalenzaga, S., Sperduti, M., & Piolino, P. (2015). Variable motor imagery training induces sleep memory consolidation and transfer improvements. Neurobiology of learning and memory, 119, 85-92.
  12. Debarnot, U., Castellani, E., & Guillot, A. (2012). Selective delayed gains following motor imagery of complex movements. Archives italiennes de biologie.‏Arch Ital Biol, 150(4):238-50
  13. Debarnot, U., Maley, L., De Rossi, D., & Guillot, A. (2010). Motor interference does not impair the memory consolidation of imagined movements. Brain and cognition, 74(1), 52-57.‏
  14. Dos Santos, J. J., Bastos, F. H., De Oliveira Souza, T., & Corrêa, U. C. (2014). Contextual interference effect depends on the amount of time separating acquisition and testing. Advances in physical education, 4 (2): 102-109.
  15. Douvis, S.J. (2005). Variable practice in learning the forehand drive in tennis. Perceptual and motor skills, 101,531-545.
  16. Guadagnoli, M. A., & Lee, T.D (2004). " Challenge point a frame work for conceptualizing the effect of various practice conditions in motor learning" .journal of motor behavior, 30, 2: 212-224.
  17. Guillot, A., & Collet, C. (2008). Construction of the motor imagery integrative model in sport: a review and theoretical investigation of motor imagery use. International review of sport and exercise psychology, 1(1), 31-44.‏
  18. Hill, S., Tononi, G., Ghilardi, A.F. (2008). Sleep improves the variability of motor performance. Brain res bull, 76: 605–611.
  19. Hoffman, K., Mcnaughton, B. (2002). Coordinated reactivation of distributed memory traces in primate neocortex. Science, 297:2070–2073.
  20. Hosseini, S.S., Rostamkhani, H., Niloo, Z., Lotfi, N. (2010). comparison of the effect of balanced, subjective and combined exercises on the balance of healthy elderly men; research in rehabilitation sciences, 6(2): 112-132.
  21. Janacsek, K., Fiser, J., Nemeth, D. (2012). The best time to acquire new skills: agerelated differences in implicit sequence learning across the human lifespan. Sci. 15, 496e505.
  22. Kantak, S. S., Sullivan, K. J., Fisher, B. E., Knowlton, B. J., & Winstein, C. J. (2011). Transfer of motor learning engages specific neural substrates during motor memory consolidation dependent on the practice structure. Journal of motor behavior, 43(6), 499-507.‏
  23. Kantak, S.S., Winstein, C.J. (2012). Learning-performance distinction and memory processes for motor skills: a focused review and perspective. Behav. Brain res. 228, 219e231.
  24. Keetch, K.M., & Lee, T. (2007)."The effects of self-regulated andexperimental –imposed practice schedules on motor learning for tasks of varying difficulty" . Research quarterly for exercise and sport, 18, 5, 476-486.
  25. Knackstedt, M.S. (2011). Optimal timing of a pettlep mental imagery intervention on a dart throwing task. Directed by dr. Jennifer l. Etnier. 69 pp.
  26. Lugassy, D. (2018). Consolidation of complex motor skill learning: evidence for a delayed offline process. Sleep; 41(9):12-27.
  27. Lee, T. D., & Magill, R. A. (1983). The locus of contextual interference in motor-skill acquisition. Journal of experimental psychology: learning, memory, and cognition, 730–746.
  28. Lotze, M., & Halsband, U. (2006). Motor imagery. Journal of physiology – paris, 99,386–395.
  29. Lundbye-Jensen, J., Petersen, T.H., Rothwell, J.C., Nielsen, J.B. (2011). Interference in ballistic motor learning: specificity and role of sensory error signals. Plos one 6, e17451.
  30. Mackay, D. G. (1981). The problem of rehearsal or mental practice». Journal of motor behavior, 13(4): 274-285.
  31. Magill, R. A., & Anderson, D. (2013). "Motor learning and control: concepts and applications" (10 ed.): mcgraw-hill New York.
  32. Roig, M., Ritterband-Rosenbaum, A., lundbye-jensen, J., Nielsen, J.B (2014). Aging increases the susceptibility to motor memory interference and reduces off-line gains in motor skill learning. Neurobiology of Aging, 35 (8): 1892-1900.
  33. Mouthon, A., Ruffieux, J., Wälchli, M., Keller, M., Taube, W. (2015). Taskdependent changes of corticospinal excitability during observation and motor imagery of balance tasks. Neuroscience; 303:535-43.
  34. Mullany, P., Lynch, M.A. (1997). Changes in protein synthesis and synthesis of the synaptic vesicle protein, synaptophysin, in entorhinal cortex following induction of long-term potentiation in dentate gyrus: an age-related study in the rat. Neuropharmacology, 36, 973e980.
  35. Nemeth, D., Janacsek, K. (2011). The dynamics of implicit skill consolidation in young and elderly adults. J. Gerontol. B. Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 66, 15e22.
  36. Oldfield, R.C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Neuropsychologia 9, 97e113.
  37. Rahavi, R., Shojaei, M., Estiri, Z., Naghizadeh, H.(2013). The effect of two types of interference before performance and during performance on learning of generalized motor program and parameter in basketball fundamental skills. Development and Motor Learning (Harakat). 5(2):107-124. [in Persian].
  38. Reber, A.S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: an essay on the cognitive Oxford university press, New York.
  39. Remy, F., Wenderoth, N, Lipkens, K., Swinnen, S.P. (2010). Dual-task interference during initial learning of a new motor task results from competition for the same brain areas. Neuropsychologia 48, 2517e2527.
  40. Rioult-Pedotti, M.S., Friedman, D., Donoghue, J.P. (2000). Learning-induced ltp in neocortex. Science 290, 533e536.
  41. Shamsipour Dehkordi, P., Abdoli, B., Ashayeri, H., And Namazizadeh, M. (2014). The effect of different training intervals on the processing of consolidation based on promotion of hidden motor memory. Journal of shahrekord university of medical sciences, 16 (3), 95-107.[in Persian].
  42. Shea, J. B., & Zimny, S. T. (1988). Knowledge incorporation in motor representation. In o. G. Meijer & k. Roth (eds.), complex movement behavior: ‘‘the’’ motor-action controversy (pp. 289–314). Amsterdam: north-holland.
  43. Song, S. (2009). Consciousness and the consolidation of motor learning. Behavioural brain research, 196(2), 180-186.‏
  44. Stephan, M.A., Meier, B., Orosz, A., Cattapan-Ludewig, K., Kaelin-Lang, A.(2009). Interference during the implicit learning of two different motor sequences. Exp. Brain res. 196, 253e261.
  45. Tomassini, V., Jbabdi, S., Kincses, Z.T., Bosnell, R., Douaud, G., Pozzilli, C., Matthews, P.M., Johansen-Berg, H. (2011). Structural and functional bases For individual differences in motor learning. Hbm 32, 494e508.
  46. Thürer, B., Weber, D., Born, J., Stein , T. (2018). Variable training but not sleep improves consolidation of motor adaptation. Sci Rep, 8(1): 15977.
  47. Voelcker-Rehage, C.(2008). Motor-skill learning in older adults: a review of studies on age-related differences. Eur. Rev. Aging phys. Act. 5, 5e16.
  48. Williams, S. E., Cumming, J., Ntoumanis, N., Nordin-Bates, S. M., Ramsey, R., & Hall, C. (2012). Further validation and development of the movement imagery questionnaire. Journal of sport & exercise psychology, 34, 621-646.‏
  49. Wrisberg, C. A., & Pein, R. L. (2002). "Note on learners' control of the frequency of model presentation during skill acquisition". Perceptual & motor skills, 94(3), pp. 792-794.
  50. Wu, W., & Magill, Ra (2005). “Allowing learners to choose: self-regulated practice schedules for learning multiple movement patterns" . Journal of sport & exercise psychology, 27, s161
  51. Zach, N., Inbar, D., Grinvald, Y., Vaadia, E.(2012). Single neurons in m1 and premotor cortex directly reflect behavioral interference. Plos one 7, e32986.
  52. Ziemann, U., Ilic, T.V., Pauli, C., Meintzschel, F., Ruge, D. (2004). Learning modifies subsequent induction of long-term potentiation-like and long-term depressionlike plasticity in human motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 24, 1666e1672.